eduzhai > Socail Sciences > Psychology >

Relationship between ABO blood group and personality in large-scale survey in Japan

  • sky
  • (0) Download
  • 20211101
  • Save
https://www.eduzhai.net International Journal of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences 2021, 11(1): 6-12 DOI: 10.5923/j.ijpbs.20211101.02 Relationship between ABO Blood Type and Personality in a Large-scale Survey in Japan Masayuki Kanazawa Human Sciences ABO Center, Tokyo, Japan Abstract The influence of genetic factors on personality has been actively studied for several decades. However, there is no scientific consensus about the specific genes involved, since the previous studies using multiple-item measures yielded inconsistent results. In this study, we conducted an analysis based on the phenotype of the ABO gene using single-item measures with a large amount of data. The result of our large-scale survey (N = 3,750) showed that respondents displayed the personality traits corresponding to their own blood type more strongly than respondents who had different blood types did. This finding was consistent across all traits, and all differences were statistically significant. In our survey, the same differences in scores were found in the groups who had no knowledge of blood type personality theory, although the values were smaller. Meanwhile, the sample in this study was limited to Japanese populations. Additional research using a large, more global dataset is needed. Keywords Blood type, Personality, Big five, Large-scale survey 1. Introduction that directly examines the association between genes and personality requires further investigation. 1.1. Background In 2017, a meta-analysis of studies involving 260,861 The influence of genetic factors on personality has been actively studied for several decades. Currently, the percentage is considered to be about 50% [1-3]. In order to improve accuracy, very large amounts of data such as various behavioral patterns and reactions are needed. Thus, the time, effort, and expense of examining over 20,000 subjects was published [6]. The researchers re-analyzed an association between the “Big Five” personality test and genes, and found that 6 genes affected human personality. However, the maximum magnitude, or the coefficient of determination, was as low as 0.04%. This value is usually considered to be an error. human genes would have to be extremely large. Even if all Nevertheless, among the many human genes, ABO blood those data were available, it would be very difficult to analyze these “big data” with conventional methods and approaches, and it would be especially difficult to obtain coherent and reliable results. An example is the relationship between variations of serotonin transporter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and subjective feelings of happiness. There are three types of 5-HTTLPR gene: the longest type, “long-long”; the shortest type, “short-short”; and the intermediate “long-short”, with individual and ethnic differences. A study of American people (N = 2,574) found that the ones who had “long-long” type of gene reported the highest subjective sense of well-being [4]. On the contrary, a study of Japanese people type is one of the exceptions. Therefore, hundreds of studies have been conducted to date, and several studies have reported differences, mainly in Japan, Korea, China and Taiwan. One reason is that these studies usually used single-item measures, whereas the previous studies of genetic factors used multiple-item measures such as personality tests. Another reason is that many people in those countries know their ABO blood type, due to blood typing at birth, conscription, and blood donation, among other reasons. However, the type information available in self-reports in these studies is limited to phenotypes (A, B, O, AB), and does not include genotypes (AA, AO, BO, BB, OO, AB). (N = 92) showed the opposite result, with “short-short” 1.2. Blood Type Personality Theory having the highest score [5]. Thus, much of the research The relationship between blood type and personality is * Corresponding author: mkana@kfx.biglobe.ne.jp (Masayuki Kanazawa) studied at an international scale, and the first academic examination using statistics as a base was conducted in 1927, Received: Feb. 25, 2021; Accepted: Mar. 6, 2021; Published: Mar. 15, 2021 Published online at https://www.eduzhai.net by Furukawa, a Japanese educational psychologist [7-12]. However, the paradigm that most influences present-day International Journal of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences 2021, 11(1): 6-12 7 research is from a Japanese book [13] published in 1971 by Nomi, a Japanese independent researcher [9,11-12]. Nomi’s research further developed Furukawa's theory (Table 1). He adopted the multiple method approach which consisted of questionnaires on the traits of people's behavior and mindset, surveys of blood type distribution for various occupations and groups, and observations of human behavior and statistical analyses. He also suggested an association with disease and physical constitution; for example, individuals with type B blood would be resistant to certain types of cancer [14-15]. Table 1. Blood Type and Its Major Traits of Personality (Nomi) Blood Type A B O AB Personality Cautious about new actions, also pursue stability, but sometimes obsessed and burst into a rage. Desire for molting. Always try to improve. Take a brave action in an emergency. Go step by step after convinced. Formula-like. Prudent judgement, although draw the clear line. Strongly suppressive outside vs violent inside. Recover slowly after got hurt. Concentrate on one at a time. Look for a life with much freedom. Particularly dislike rules or formulas. Do not hesitate to take new actions. Tend to be absorbed in strongly interested things. Multitasking and go overboard. Quick and flexible judgements. Pragmatic and do not draw the line. Emphasize scientific accuracy and validity. Feelings sway, moody. Frankly express anger or sorrow. Purpose oriented. Head straight for a target. Great achievement power. However, do not endure meaninglessness. Give up early if no good. Weigh losses against gains correctly. Hold a belief. Articulate and logical, but somewhat straight. Simple minded in part. Emotions are usually stable and do not linger. Deeply moved. Lose heart when cornered. Good reflexes, business-like efficiency. Quick and easy understanding. Rationality itself. Good critic and analyst, multi-angle interpretation. Duality with a calm, cool stable side and an easily disturbed side with sentimental fragility. Able to do everything accurately. Good at designing but not cleaning up. Smiling and soft, but keep a certain distance from others. 1.3. Results of Previous Studies Questionnaires by Japanese psychologists reported the following images of each blood type. Yamazaki and Sakamoto conducted a survey of 177 female undergraduate students on 24 personality traits in the annual opinion poll by JNN Data Bank, a department of Tokyo Broadcasting Corporation, one of the major television networks in Japan [18]. Sato, Miyazaki and Watanabe surveyed 197 undergraduate students and the results of free responses [19]. Watanabe extracted personality traits of each blood type from multiple books and asked 102 undergraduate students whether they were applicable [20]. These results are shown in Table 2. In general, the personality traits studied by Japanese psychologists were consistent with those of Nomi (Table 1). Table 2. Blood Type and Its Major Images of Personality (Psychologists) Blood Type # Personality I am careful when doing something. 1 I am serious when I should be so. I am often worried about things. A 2 Meticulous, Nervous, Serious 3 Considerate, careful about everything, Polit, Esteem principle than practice, Responsible, Introvert I often change my mind. 1 I am not particular about things. I am not worried by what I was told. B 2 Self-paced, Individual, Lukewarm, Egoistic, Self-centered, Optimistic, Pleasant 3 Lack of prudence, Self-paced, less influenced by the surroundings, Active and curious; Optimistic I have a lot of friends. 1 I am ready to keep company with everyone. I cannot be quiet and I make merry when I am delighted. O 2 Big-hearted, Laid back, Unassuming 3 Pleasant, Positive, person of action, Passionate, Show the utmost courage and guts for the target I do not change my idea after I present it. 1 I make an effort toward my goal. I sometimes burst into a rage. AB 2 Dual personality, Two-faced, Oddball, Hard to understand 3 Moody and sometimes seen as dual personality, Cool and businesslike, Avoid close human relations Note. #1: Yamazaki and Sakamoto [18]; #2: Sato, Miyazaki and Watanabe [19]; #3: Watanabe [20]. 1.4. Results of Personality Tests Questionnaire-based personality assessments are frequently used in psychology and consist of answering multiple questions regarding self-reported personality traits; responses are then integrated into several personality factors by statistical processing. In theory, this means that the self-reported answer will either directly or indirectly appear in the result. Although there are many academic studies on the relationship between blood type and personality, the inconsistency among results [7-9,12,20-32] has led to an endless academic controversy about whether the relationship has been scientifically confirmed. Many studies examined the association between blood type and personality using the “Big Five” personality test, which has been extensively present in contemporary psychology. However, none of these results has been consistently replicated [9,12,22-24,30-32]. The Big Five personality test, as the name implies, comprehensively describes personality using five factors called the Big Five [33-34]. The model collects vocabularies from dictionaries and traditional personality tests, as well as re-analyses of personality scales, and the five factors were extracted through factor analysis. Thus, 8 Masayuki Kanazawa: Relationship between ABO Blood Type and Personality in a Large-scale Survey in Japan the Big Five does not assume any background theory; it is characterized by the bottom-up process and personality is comprehensively captured by the five factors [35]. These five factors are usually called Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness. The NEO-PI-R, commonly used as a Big Five personality test, consists of 240 question items, each of which is rated using a five-point scale [36]. 1.5. Self-fulfilling Prophecy The term “self-fulfilling prophecy” refers to the phenomenon in which a person who believes in a prophecy learns to act in accordance with the prophecy, thereby bringing the prophecy to being. An example that psychologists have studied is astrology [37-39]. If a person's original personality matches his/her astrological sign, that tendency becomes stronger. Even if the personality and the sign do not initially match, the personality moves toward what has been predicted by the astrological sign. In Japan, Korea and Taiwan, roughly half of the people believe the relationship between blood type and personality is legitimate [10,12,16-17,22,40-42]. Logically, the self-fulfilling findings among astrology suggest that one's personality would change in a direction that conforms to the descriptions suggested by his/her blood type. Based on this self-fulfilling prophecy hypothesis, several large-scale surveys in Japan using the items in Table 2, designed or analyzed by academic researchers since 1990, clearly confirms this phenomenon. Archetypical sample sizes of these were 1,300 [41], 6,660 [42], 11,766 [43], 32,347 [18,44-45], and over 200,000 (our estimation; the exact number was not specified in this report, although it alluded that the size was much larger than preceding surveys) [46]. Nevertheless, researchers have confirmed no coherent statistical difference in participants without knowledge of astrology or blood type [40-46]. In other words, the current scientific consensus is that these differences are self-fulfilling phenomena induced by the “contamination by knowledge” [9,18,22,27,37-46]. However, there was little testing of whether the questions used in these studies were suitable for examining differences among less knowledgeable respondents. 1.6. Contemporary Trends and Issues After 2000, a growing number of studies proved the previously questioned link between blood type and physical constitution, with the exception being in the weak gastrointestinal tract: this demonstration proposed a new approach to medicine [47-48]. For example, several studies found that individuals with type A were more susceptible to COVID-19, whereas individuals with type O were less susceptible [49-51]. There have also been several studies on biological factors, which investigated whether physical constitution affected personality. An American researcher evaluated the both the ABO genotype and its phenotype, and the linkage of disequilibrium between the ABO and DBH genes [52-53]. A Korean 2011 EEG analysis study rated type O as having higher scores in awareness and stress resistance than other types [54]. In 2015, a Japanese study using DNA testing methods found that ABO genes of 1,427 participants tended to be persistent in type A, as predicted by blood type personality theory [55]. This study used the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI) which built a model for temperament with a physiological basis in the background, unlike the Big Five. The TCI, a top-down personality model, is often used to examine genetic dispositions [56-57]. The test consists of 240 items using a yes-no scale rating. Cloninger hypothesized that personality consists of traits that are hereditary and stable throughout life. The TCI consists of seven dimensions, including 4 temperament dimensions (Novelty Seeking, Harm Avoidance, Reward Dependence, and Persistence) and 3 character dimensions (Self-directedness, Cooperativeness and Self-transcendence). Three of the temperament dimensions have been hypothesized to be associated with monoamine neurotransmitters. Novelty seeking has been hypothesized to be associated with dopaminergic, harm avoidance with serotonergic, and reward dependence with noradrenergic. With the progress of information technology, conducting crowdsourced surveys has become much easier. Therefore, a larger amount of data can be obtained at a lower cost and in a shorter period. Most Japanese people know their blood type since, until recently, it had been common practice to test the blood type of newborns. This means that analyzing the effect of the phenotype eliminates physiological type testing, and therefore our survey can be completed via the Internet. 2. Participants and Methods 2.1. Participants The data used in this study was collected by the means of crowdsourcing. The survey was conducted in 2020 with a sample size of 4,000 Japanese individuals between the ages of 20 and 59. The participants were asked to rate a total of 8 items representing 4 blood type (A, B, O, AB) traits, each with scores from 1 to 7 for their own personality traits (the larger the number, the more fitting the trait). Respondents were also asked which blood type they thought these 8 items would correspond to. Additionally, scores of 1 to 3 denoting the strength of the relationship (the larger the number, the closer the relationship) and 1 to 4 of their own familiarity with blood type personality theory (the larger the number, the more knowledgeable the participant was). We tried to follow those methods of psychological personality testing, and deliberately selected the most suitable traits (Table 3) that would certainly produce the differences: images of traits were consistent to the preceding academic studies, they showed large differences in the academic studies and their means were close to 50%, they did not show extreme values, and they were consistent to the International Journal of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences 2021, 11(1): 6-12 9 preceding surveys of other studies. Table 3. Blood Type and Its Major Traits of Personality (Nomi) Blood Type A B O AB Personality Meticulous, Nervous Self-paced, Self-centered Big-hearted, Laid back Hard to be understood, Dual personality With the intention of avoiding ethical issues, question items on blood type traits of this study were extracted from reviewed academic articles (Table 2). These items were checked again at the previously stated crowdsourcing company which has passed the Japanese privacy review process (JIS Q 15001). The company confirmed that there was no problem with the question items. It provides customers with anonymized data and obtains informed consent from participants (respondents) prior to its surveys. Each participant’s blood type of was determined by self-report, because most Japanese people know their blood type, as previously mentioned. In this survey, 250 out of all the 4,000 respondents, or 6.3% of the total, did not know their blood type. Thereby the data of the remaining 3,750 participants with known blood types were used. All samples were equally allocated by age in 10-year increments, and gender. 2.2. Statistical Analysis We focused our analysis on whether personality self-fulfillment was occurring or not, using data from participants who “have no knowledge of blood type personality” or “do not believe in the relationship” (hereinafter abbreviated as “no-knowledge group”). This survey had 1,474 participants from the “no-knowledge group”; we believed these participants would be free of the aforementioned “contamination by knowledge.” Our analytical methods were as follows: Analysis 1: ANOVA of blood type and personality of the whole participants Analysis 2: ANOVA of blood type and personality of the “no-knowledge” group We set the alpha level to 0.05. ANOVA were used to analyze blood type traits. The distributions of participants’ blood types were almost equal to the Japanese average. 3. Results 3.1. Analysis 1: ANOVA of Blood Type and Personality of the Whole Participants All traits showed the same results as those shown for blood type traits in the preceding psychology studies (Table 2). All traits were statistically significant at p < 0.05 (Table 4). All the responses were consistent with the most common images of blood types that respondents expected. On question item “Do you think blood type and personality are related?”, 10.2% of the respondents answered “related,” 41.4% answered “somewhat related,” 32.9% answered “not related,” and 15.6% answered “I don't know.” On question item “Do you know the characteristics and compatibility of blood types?”, 4.7% answered “I know a lot,” 36.0% answered “I know some,” 40.2% answered “I know a little,” and 19.1% answered “I don't know at all.” Table 4. ANOVA of the Whole Participants Blood Type Traits A B O AB Average Scores by Blood Type A B O AB (N=1383) (N=831) (N=1115) (N=421) 4.53 4.03 4.03 4.21 4.22 4.57 4.25 4.30 4.03 4.14 4.47 4.05 3.76 3.86 3.70 4.33 p <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 Note. Highest scores that match with blood type characteristics, and p < 0.05 are highlighted in bold. 3.2. Analysis 2: ANOVA of Blood Type and Personality of the “No-knowledge” Group All traits showed the same results as those shown for blood type traits in the preceding psychology studies (Table 2). All traits were statistically significant at p < 0.05 (Table 5). Table 5. ANOVA of the “No-knowledge” Group Blood Type Traits A B O AB Average Scores by Blood Type A B O AB (N=560) (N=343) (N=419) (N=152) 4.29 4.14 4.11 4.01 4.27 4.60 4.38 4.14 4.04 4.02 4.39 4.01 3.74 3.80 3.80 4.10 p 0.031 <0.001 <0.001 0.012 Note. Highest scores that match with blood type characteristics, and p < 0.05 are highlighted in bold. 4. Discussion 4.1. Issues with Personality Tests In this study, all blood type trait items clearly showed the differences as predicted. This study measured self-reported personality traits, and since many Japanese believe in the relationship between blood type and personality, conventional personality psychology theory suggests the differences among the blood types will certainly appear, as previously mentioned. For the same reason, personality psychology is not sufficient to explain the inconsistent results that have appeared in previous research [7-9,12,20-32]. Ryu and Sohn [58] re-analyzed Cho, Suh and Ro’s result of the Big Five test of 40 items [22], and found statistically significant differences which match with blood type traits in 10 Masayuki Kanazawa: Relationship between ABO Blood Type and Personality in a Large-scale Survey in Japan 10 individual items. This means that in the case of a “personality factor” composed of multiple items, the difference by the blood type decreases ‒ few significant differences appear. Thus, the Big Five personality test can hardly detect minor differences in personality, such as those that may be caused by a single gene, at least theoretically. If the relationship between ABO blood type and personality exists, respondents are expected to exhibit the personality traits corresponding to their own blood type more strongly than the other types. And if these differences are inborn, not due to the self-fulfilling phenomena induced by the “contamination by knowledge,” it is also expected that the same differences in scores will be found in the groups with no knowledge on blood type traits. Thus, the lack of statistical significance in previous studies could have been a Type 2 error caused by the small sample size, multiple-item measures, or unsuitable questions. 5. Conclusions Both all blood type traits in the whole participants and all traits in the “no-knowledge group” clearly showed the differences as predicted. We found a clear relationship between blood type and the self-reported personality using 8 single question items, which matches traits previously stated. Meanwhile, the sample in this study was limited to Japanese populations. Additional research using a large, more global dataset is needed in order to address the true implications as well as to improve methodologies. [4] De Neve JE. Functional polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) in the serotonin transporter gene is associated with subjective well-being: evidence from a US nationally representative sample. J Hum Genet. 2011; 56: 456–459. DOI: 10.1038/jhg.2011.39. [5] Matsunaga M, Isowa T, Yamakawa K, Ohira H. Association between the serotonin transporter polymorphism (5HTTLPR) and subjective happiness level in Japanese adults. Psychology of Well-Being: Theory, Research and Practice. 2013; 3: 5. DOI: 10.1186/2211-1522-3-5. [6] Lo MT, Hinds DA, Tung JY, Franz C, Fan CC, Wang Y et al. Genome-wide analyses for personality traits identify six genomic loci and show correlations with psychiatric disorders. Nat Genet. 2017; 49: 152–156. DOI: 10.1038/ng.3736. [7] Furukawa T. A Study of temperament by means of human blood groups. Japanese Journal of Psychology. 1927; 2(4): 612-634. DOI:10.4992/jjpsy.2.612. [8] Furukawa T. A study of temperament and blood groups. Journal of Social Psychology 1927; 1: 494–509. DOI: 10.1080/00224545.1930.9714153. [9] Rogers M, Glendon AI. Blood type and personality. Pers Individ Dif. 2003; 34: 1099–1112. DOI: 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00101-0. [10] Sato T, Watanabe Y. Psychological studies on blood-typing in Japan. Japanese Psychological Review. 1992; 35: 234–268. DOI: 10.24602/sjpr.35.2_234. [11] Wang JQ. Blood type and personality. 2004; Beijing: Beijing Institute of Technology Press. [12] Wu K, Lindsted KD, Lee JW. Blood type and the five factors of personality in Asia. Pers Individ Dif. 2005; 384: 797-808. DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2004.06.004. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS [13] Nomi M. Understanding compatibility through blood type. 1971; Tokyo: Seishun Publishing. The author is grateful to Chieko Ichikawa, Director of the Human Sciences ABO Center, for her support. The author also thanks Professor Qinglai Meng of Oregon State University, as well as Fred Wong, co-founder of AI Hong Kong Limited, for their kind comments and suggestions. [14] Nomi M. New blood type humanics. 1978; Tokyo: Sankei Publishing. [15] Shirasa T, Iguchi T. An introduction to research on blood type and personality. 1993; Tokyo: Kawashima Shoten. [16] Fuji Television Networks. Nippon no daimondai (November 8, 2015). Tokyo, Japan. REFERENCES [17] NHK Broadcasting Culture Research Institute. A survey on “scientific technology and live”. Tokyo: NHK Service Center (NHK Broadcasting Culture Research Institute, 1986). [1] Ando J, Suzuki A, Yamagata S, Kijima N, Maekawa H, Ono Y, Jang KL. Genetic and environmental structure of Cloninger's temperament and character dimensions. J Pers Disord. 2004; 18(4): 379-393. DOI: 10.1521/pedi.18.4.379.40345. [18] Yamazaki K, Sakamoto A. Self-fulfillment phenomenon generated by blood-typical personality stereotypes: time-series analysis of nation-wide survey. 32nd Annual conference of Japanese society of social psychology. 1991; 32: 289-292. [2] Chabris CF, Lee JJ, Cesarini D, Benjamin DJ, Laibson DI. The Fourth Law of Behavior Genetics. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2015; 24(4) 304–312. DOI: 10.1177/0963721415580430. [19] Sato T, Miyazaki S, Watanabe Y. An examination of the blood type personality relationship theory (3) from stereotype to prejudice. 2nd convention of Japan Society of Development Psychology. 1991; 147. [3] Robert Plomin R, DeFries JC, Knopik VS, Jenae M, Neiderhiser JM. Top 10 replicated findings from behavioral genetics. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2016; 11(1): 3-23. DOI: 10.1177/1745691615617439. [20] Watanabe Y. The roles of prototype and exemplar in the formation of the “blood type stereotype”. Japanese Statistical study on personality differences by blood type. Japanese Journal of Social Psychology. 1994; 10(2): 77-86. International Journal of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences 2021, 11(1): 6-12 11 DOI: 10.14966/jssp.KJ00003724631. [21] Cattell RB, Boutourline HY, Hundleby JD. Blood groups and personality traits. American Journal of Human Genetics. 1964; 16: 397–402. Retrieved from https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/14250420/. [22] Cho SH, Suh EKM, Ro YJ. Beliefs about blood types and traits and their reflections in self-reported personality. Korean Journal of Social and Personality Psychology. 2005; 19(4): 33-47. Retrieved from http://kiss.kstudy.com/thesis/thesis-vi ew.asp?key=2498184. [23] Cramer KM, Imaike E. Personality, blood type, and the five-factor model. Pers Individ Dif. 2002; 32: 621–626. DOI: 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00064-2. [24] Flegr J, Preiss M, Klose J. Toxoplasmosis-associated difference in intelligence and personality in men depends on their Rhesus blood group but not ABO blood group. PLoS One. 2013; 8, e61272. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0061272. [36] Costa PT Jr, McCrae RR. The NEO-P-I-R professional manual: Revised NEO Personality Inventory. (NEO-P-I-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (Neo-FFI). 1992; Odessa, Fla: Psychological Assessment Resources. [37] Eysenck HJ, Nias DKB. Astrology: Science or superstition?. 1982; London: Curtis Brown Temple Smith. [38] Glick P, Snyder M. Self-Fulfilling Prophecy: The psychology of belief in astrology. Humanist. 1986; 46(3): 20-24. [39] Snyder M. When Belief Creates Reality. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. 1984; 18, 247-305. DOI: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60146-X. [40] Kamise Y, Matsui Y. Functions and structure of blood-group stereotype. Bulletin of University of the Sacred Heart. 1994; 82: 90-111. [41] Yamaoka S. A psychology book for not to become a useless adult. 2001; Tokyo: Brain Publishing. [25] Gupta S. Blood groups and personality characteristics. Pers Individ Dif. 1990; 3, 317–318. DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(90)90246-N. [26] Jogawar VV. Personality correlates of human blood groups. Pers Individ Dif. 1983; 4: 215–216. DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(83)90026-0. [27] Kim BM, Lee DM, Lee SH, Gim WS. Blood-type distribution. Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications. 2007; 373: 533-540. DOI: 10.1016/j.physa.2006.05.027. [28] Lester D, Gatto J. Personality and blood group. Pers Individ Dif. 1987; 8: 267. DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(87)90184-X. [42] Yamaoka S. Discrimination and delusion of blood type personality divination. 18th Annual Meeting of Japan Society of Personality Psychology. 2009; 18, 11. DOI: 10.24534/amjspp.18.0_11. [43] Matsui Y. A statistical analysis of personality difference by blood type. Bulletin of Tokyo Metropolitan Tachikawa Junior College. 1991; 24: 51-54. [44] Yamazaki K, Sakamoto A. Self-fulfillment phenomenon generated by blood-typical personality stereotypes: time-series analysis of nation-wide survey 2. 33rd Annual conference of Japanese society of social psychology. 1992; 33: 342-345. [29] Mao X, Xu M, Mu S, Ma Y, He M. Study on relationship between human ABO blood A group and type A behavior pattern. Hua Xi Yi Ke Da Xue Xue Bao. 1991; 22(1): 93-96. [30] Shimizu T, Ishikawa M. Relationships between ABO blood types and personality: Measurement by the Five Factor Model. Kozokoseishugi-kenkyu. 2011; 5: 78-91. [31] Sharifi M, Ahmadian H, Jalali A. The relationship between the big five personality factors with blood types in Iranian university students. Journal of Chemical and Pharmaceutical Research. 2015; 7(5): 233-240. Retrieved from https://www.jocpr.com/abstract/the-relationship-between-the -big-five-personality-factors-with-blood-types-inrniranian-u niversity-students-7553.html. [45] Sakamoto A, Yamazaki K. Blood-typical personality stereotypes and self-fulfilling prophecy: A natural experiment with time-series data of 1978-1988. Progress in Asian Social Psychology. 2004; 4: 239-262. Seoul: Kyoyook-KwahakSa. [46] Muto C, Nagashima M, Harada J, Abe S, Furuya Y, Kamizon K, Konishi Y. A demonstrative and critical study on pseudo-science for scientific literacy construction at teacher education course. Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research FY2011 Final Research Report (Japan). 2012. https://kaken. nii.ac.jp/en/grant/KAKENHI-PROJECT-22650191/. [47] Ewald DR, Sumner SC. Blood type biochemistry and human disease. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Syst Biol Med. 2016; 8(6): 517-535. DOI: 10.1002/wsbm.1355. [32] Alsadi R. Personality traits and their relationship with blood groups among of Palestinian university students. International Journal of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences. 2020; 10(2): 34-42. DOI: 10.5923/j.ijpbs.20201002.02. [33] Goldberg LR. An alternative “description of personality”: the big-five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1990; 59(6): 1216-29. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.59.6.1216. [48] Risch NJ. Searching for genetic determinants in the new millennium. Nature. 2000; 405(6788): 847-56. DOI: 10.1038/35015718. [49] Barnkob MB, Pottegård A, Støvring H, Haunstrup TM, Homburg K, Larsen R, Hansen MB, Titlestad K. Reduced prevalence of SARS-CoV-2 infection in ABO blood group O. Blood Adv. 2020; 4(20): 4990-4993. DOI: 10.1182/bloodadvances.2020002657. [34] Goldberg LR. The development of markers for the big-five factor structure. Psychological Assessment. 1992; 4: 26-42. DOI: 10.1037/1040-3590.4.1.26. [35] Kunisato Y, Yamaguchi A, Suzuki S. Cloninger's Temperament and Character Model and the Big Five. Japanese Journal of Personality, 2008; 16: 8, 323-324. DOI: 10.2132/personality.16.324. [50] Hoiland RL, Fergusson NA, Mitra AR, Griesdale DEG, Devine DV, Stukas S et al. The association of ABO blood group with indices of disease severity and multiorgan dysfunction in COVID-19. Blood Adv. 2020; 4(20): 4981–4989. DOI: 10.1182/bloodadvances.2020002623. [51] Severe Covid-19 GWAS Group. Genomewide association study of severe Covid-19 with respiratory failure. N Engl J 12 Masayuki Kanazawa: Relationship between ABO Blood Type and Personality in a Large-scale Survey in Japan Med. 2020; 383(16): 1522-1534. DOI: 10.1056/NEJMoa2020283. [52] Hobgood DK. Personality traits of aggression-submissiveness and perfectionism associate with ABO blood groups through catecholamine activities. Med Hypotheses. 2011; 77: 294–300. DOI: 10.1016/j.mehy.2011.04.039. [53] Hobgood DK. ABO group B is associated with personality traits through linkage disequilibrium with low activity dopamine beta hydroxylase. Hypothesis in the Life Sciences. 2011; 1: 1–8. http://www.hy-ls.org/index.php/hyls/article/vie w/4/0.html. [54] Kim CS, Yi SG. A Study on the effects of one's blood type on emotional character and antistress of adults. Journal of the Korea Academia-Industrial cooperation Society. 2011; 12(6): 2554-2560. DOI:10.5762/KAIS.2011.12.6.2554. [55] Tsuchimine S, Saruwatari J, Kaneda A, Yasui-Furukori N. ABO blood type and personality traits in healthy Japanese subjects. PLoS ONE. 2015; 10(5): e0126983. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0126983. [56] Cloninger CR. A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants. A proposal. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1987; 44: 573-588. DOI: 10.1001/archpsyc.1987.01800180093014. [57] Kijima N, Saito R, Takeuchi M, Yoshino I, Ono Y, Kato M, Kitamura T. Cloninger’s seven-factor model of temperament and character and Japanese version of Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI). Kikan Seishinka Shindangaku. 1996; 7: 379-399. Retrieved from https://www.institute-of-m ental-health.jp/thesis/pdf/thesis-06/thesis-06-04.pdf. [58] Ryu SI, Sohn YW. A Review of Sociocultural, Behavioral, Biochemical Analyzes on ABO Blood-Groups Typology. Korean Journal of Social and Personality Psychology. 2007; 21(3): 27-55. DOI: 10.21193/kjspp.2007.21.3.003. Copyright © 2021 The Author(s). Published by Scientific & Academic Publishing This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution International License (CC BY). http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/

... pages left unread,continue reading

Document pages: 7 pages

Please select stars to rate!

         

0 comments Sign in to leave a comment.

    Data loading, please wait...
×